شکل¬گیری و تحول باورهای کودکان 6 -3 ساله دربارة دانش و دانستن
محورهای موضوعی :زهرا تنها 1 * , پروین کدیور 2 , محمدحسین عبدالهی 3 , جعفر حسنی 4
1 - دانشگاه آزاد اسلامی واحد خرم آباد
2 - دانشگاه خوارزمی
3 - دانشگاه خوارزمی
4 - دانشگاه خوارزمی
کلید واژه: معرفت¬, شناسی شخصینظریة ذهنباور غلطفرهنگ,
چکیده مقاله :
پژوهش حاضر با هدف بررسی تحول باورها در سنین کودکی با درنظر گرفتن نظریة ذهن انجام شد. بدین منظور، تعداد 75 کودک در سنین 6-3 سال از مهدکودک های شهر تهران انتخاب شدند. برای هر کودک دو تکلیفِ باورِ غلط و دو تکلیف معرفت شناسی شخصی در نظر گرفته شد. نتایج نشان دادند که بین معرفت شناسی شخصی، نظریة ذهن، و سن رابطة معنادارِ مثبت دیده می شود. نتایج تحلیل رگرسیون نشان دادند که معرفت شناسیِ شخصی از طریق سن و نظریة ذهن قابل پیش بینی است. بر این اساس، کودکانِ سنین بالاتر نمره های بالاتری در معرفت شناسی شخصی کسب کردند، به گونه ای که تقریباً %31 از واریانس معرفت شناسی شخصی از طریق سن تبیین شد. همچنین، کودکانی که نمره های بالاتری در معرفت شناسی شخصی کسب کردند نمره های بالاتری در توانایی نظریة ذهن به دست آوردند. دو متغیر سن و نظریة ذهن همراه با هم، %5/32 از واریانس معرفت شناسی شخصی را تبیین کردند. به علاوه، مرحله انتقال به دوگانه نگری در نمونة مورد بررسی، مشاهده نشد.
Development of individual beliefs about nature of knowledge and knowing has been one of the important educational research topics in recent decades. The present study aimed to examine belief’s development with regard to the theory of mind at early ages. Hence, a sample of 75 children aged 3-6 years were selected from Tehran kindergartens. For each child, two False Belief Tasks and two Personal Epistemology Tasks were used. The results showed that there was a significant positive relationship between personal epistemology, theory of mind, and age. Results of regression analysis showed that personal epistemology was predictable by age and theory of mind. Accordingly, the older children got higher scores in personal epistemology, and approximately 31% of the variance in personal epistemology was explained by age. Also, children with higher scores in personal epistemology obtained higher scores in theory of mind, and age and theory of mind together explained 32.5% of the variance of personal epistemology. Moreover, the transition to dualism was not observed in the studied sample.
جوكار، بهرام؛ ساماني، سيامك و فولادچنگ، محبوبه. (1379). ديدگاه¬گيري در كودكان پيشدبستاني: بررسي مقايسهاي درون¬فردي و بين¬فردي. روان¬شناسي و علوم تربيتي، 5(2)، 136-159.
عبداله¬زاده¬رافی، مهدی؛ بهرامی، هادی؛ میرزمانی، سیدمحمود؛ صالحی، مسعود و حسن¬زاده¬اول، مریم. (1389). تحول نظریة ذهن دانش آموزان عقب¬مانده ذهنی و ارتباط آن با تعداد خواهران و برادران. توانبخشی، 38، 19-27.
کدیور، پروین؛ تنها، زهرا و فرزاد، ولی¬اله. (1390). رابطه باورهای معرفت¬شناختی، رویکردهای یادگیری، و تفکر تأملی با پیشرفت تحصیلی. مجلة روان¬شناسی، 16(3)، 251-265. Astington, J. W., Pelletier, J., & Homer, B. (2002). Theory of mind and epistemological development: The relation between children's second order false-belief understanding and their ability to reason about evidence. New Ideas in Psychology, 20 (2-3), 131- 144.
Avis, J., & Harris, P. (1991). Belief-desire reasoning among Baka Children: Evidence for a Universal Conception of Mind. Child Development, 62, 460-467.
Brownlee, L., Johansson, E., Walker, S. & Scholes, L. (2017). Teaching for active citizenship: personal epistemology and practices in early education classrooms. London: Routledge
Burr, J. E. & Hofer, B. E. (2002). Personal epistemology and theory of mind: deciphering young children’s beliefs about knowledge and knowing. New Ideas in Psychology, 20, 199-224.
Chandler, M. J. & Carpendale, J. I. (1998). Inching toward a mature theory of mind. In M. D. Ferrari and R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Self-awareness: Its nature and development (148–90). New York: Guilford.
Chandler, M. J., Hallet, D., & Sokol, B. W. (2002). Competing claims about competing knowledge claims. In B. K. Hofer, and P. R. Pintrich,. (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The Psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style: Understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training. Child Development, 65, 1111-1119.
Chen, X., Dong, Q., & Zhou, H. (1997). Authoritative and authoritarian parenting practices and social and school adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21, 855-873.
Fagnant, A., & Crahay, M. (2011). Theories of mind and personal epistemology: Their interrelation and connection with the concept of metacognition. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 26, 257-271.
Ford, R. M., Driscoll, T., Shum, D., & Macaulay, C. E. (2012). Executive and theory-of-mind contributions to event-based prospective memory in children: Exploring the self-projection hypothesis. Journal of Experi-mental Child Psychology, 111, 468-489.
Frank, G., Plunkett, S. W., & Otten, M. P. (2010). Perceived parenting, self-esteem, and general self-efficacy of Iranian American adolescents. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 19(6), 738-746.
Ghorbani, N. (2003). Individualist and collectivist values: Evidence of compatibility in Iran and the U.S.A. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 431-447.
Gopnik, A., & Graf, P. (1988). Knowing how you know: Young children’s ability to identify and remember the sources of their beliefs. Child Development, 59, 1366-1371.
Greenfield, P. M., Keller, H., Fuligni, A., & Maynard, A. (2003). Cultural pathways through universal development. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 461-490.
Henning, A., Spinath, F. M., & Aschersleben, G. (2011). The link between preschooler’s executive function and theory of mind and the role of epistemic states. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 513-531.
Hofer, B. K., & Pintrich, P. R. (1997). The development of epistemological theories: Beliefs about knowledge and knowing and their relation to learning. Review of Educational Research, 67(1), 88-140.
King, P. M., & Kitchener, K. S. (1994). Developing reflective judgment: Understanding and promoting intellectual growth and critical thinking in adolescents and adults. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Kuhn, D. (2000a). Metacognitive development. Psychological Science, 9, 178-181.
Kuhn, D. (2000b). Theory of mind, metacognition, and reasoning: A life-span perspective. In P. Mitchell & K. J. Riggs (Eds.), Children's reasoning and the mind (p. 301-326). Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis (UK).
Kuhn, D., & Weinstock, M. (2002).What is epistemological thinking and why does it matter? In B. K. Hofer, , and P. R. Pintrich, (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing, Mahwah. NJ, Erlbaum.
Lalonde, C. E. & Chandler, M. J. (1995). False belief understanding goes to school: On the social-emotional consequences of coming early or late to a first theory of mind. Cognition and Emotion, 9(2-3), 167-85.
Lecce, S., Bianco, F., Devine, R. T., Hughes, C., & Banerjee, R. (2014). Promoting theory of mind during middle childhood: A training program. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 52-67.
Lin, T., Deng, F., Chai, C. S., & Tsai, C. (2013). High school students’ scientific epistemological beliefs, motivation in learning science, and their relationships: A comparative study within the Chinese culture. International Journal of Educational Development, 33(1), 37-47.
Liu, D., Wellman, H. M., Tardif, T., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2008). Theory of mind development in chinese children: A meta-analysis of false-belief understanding across cultures and languages. Developmental Psychology, 44(2), 523-531.
Liu, S. & Rohring, G. (2019). Exploring science teachers' argumentation and personal epistemology about global climate change. Research Science Education, 49, 173-189.
Lordanou, K. (2017). From theory of mind to epistemic cognition: A lifespan perspective. Frontline Learning Research, 4(5), 106-119.
Manikutty, S., Anuradha, N. S. & Hansen, K. (2007). Does culture influence learning styles in higher education? International Journal of Learning and Change, 2(1), 70-87.
Mansfield, A., & Clinchy, B. (1985). Early growth of multiplism in the child. Paper presented at the fifteenth annual symposium of the Jean Piaget society. Philadelphia, PA.
McGinnis, D. (2016). Epistemological orientations and evidence evaluation in undergraduates. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 19, 279-289.
Miscione, J. L., Marvin, R. S., O’Brien, R. G., & Greenberg, M. T. (1978). A developmental study of preschool children’s understanding of the words ‘‘know’’ and ‘‘guess’’. Child Development, 49, 1107-1113.
Montgomery, D. E. (1992). Young children’s theory of knowing: The development of a folk epistemology. Developmental Review, 12, 410-430.
Muis, K. R., Chevrier, M. & Singh, C. A. (2018). The role of epistemic emotions in personal epistemology and self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 53(3), 165-184.
O’Neill, D. K., & Gopnik, A. (1991). Young children’s ability to identify the sources of their beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 27(3), 390-397.
O’Neill, D. K., Astington, J. W., & Flavell, J. H. (1992). Young children’s understanding of the role that sensory experiences play in knowledge acquisition. Child Development, 63, 474-490.
Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three -year- old's difficaulty with false belief. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125- 137.
Perry, W. G. (1970). Forms of intellectual and ethical development in the college years: A scheme. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Rhodes, M., & Brandone, A. (2014). Three-year-olds’ theories of mind in actions and words. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1-8.
Rudy, D., & Grusec, J. E. (2001). Correlates of authoritarian parenting in individualist and collectivist cultures and implications for the socialization of values. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 202–212.
Schommer-Aikins, M. (2004). Explaining the epistemological beliefs system: Introducing the embedded systemic model and coordinated research approach. Educational Psychologist, 39(1), 19-29.
Sharifzadeh, V. (2004). Families with middle eastern roots. In E. Lynch & M. Hanson (Eds.), Developing cross-cultural competence. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Tabak, I., & Weinstock, M. (2008). A sociocultural exploration of epistemo-logical beliefs. In M. S. Khine (Ed.), knowing, knowledge and beliefs. Springer press.
Tafreshi, D., & Racine, T. (2016). Children’s interpretive theory of mind: The role of mothers’ personal epistemologies and mother-child talk about interpretation. Cognitive Development, 39, 57-70.
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655-684.
Wellman, H., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory of mind tasks. Child Development, 75, 523-541.
Wildenger, L. K., Hofer, B. K., & Burr, J. E. (2010). Epistemological development in very young knowers, In L. D. Bendixen, and F. Feucht, (Eds.), Personal epistemology in the classroom. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representations and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103-108.
Zhang, L. -F. (1999). A comparison of U.S. and Chinese university students’ cognitive development: The cross-cultural applicability of Perry’s theory. Journal of Psychology, 133, 425-439.
Zhang, T., Shao, Z., & Zhang, Y. (2016). Developmental steps in theory of mind of typical Chinese children and Chinese children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 23, 210-220.